Papers
Topics
Authors
Recent
Gemini 2.5 Flash
Gemini 2.5 Flash
38 tokens/sec
GPT-4o
59 tokens/sec
Gemini 2.5 Pro Pro
41 tokens/sec
o3 Pro
7 tokens/sec
GPT-4.1 Pro
50 tokens/sec
DeepSeek R1 via Azure Pro
28 tokens/sec
2000 character limit reached

Linking In-context Learning in Transformers to Human Episodic Memory (2405.14992v2)

Published 23 May 2024 in cs.CL and cs.LG
Linking In-context Learning in Transformers to Human Episodic Memory

Abstract: Understanding connections between artificial and biological intelligent systems can reveal fundamental principles of general intelligence. While many artificial intelligence models have a neuroscience counterpart, such connections are largely missing in Transformer models and the self-attention mechanism. Here, we examine the relationship between interacting attention heads and human episodic memory. We focus on induction heads, which contribute to in-context learning in Transformer-based LLMs. We demonstrate that induction heads are behaviorally, functionally, and mechanistically similar to the contextual maintenance and retrieval (CMR) model of human episodic memory. Our analyses of LLMs pre-trained on extensive text data show that CMR-like heads often emerge in the intermediate and late layers, qualitatively mirroring human memory biases. The ablation of CMR-like heads suggests their causal role in in-context learning. Our findings uncover a parallel between the computational mechanisms of LLMs and human memory, offering valuable insights into both research fields.

Parallels Between Induction Heads in Transformer Models and Human Episodic Memory

The paper explores the underexamined connection between Transformer models, specifically attention heads in Transformers, and human episodic memory. By focusing on "induction heads", components critical for in-context learning (ICL) in Transformer-based LLMs, the research introduces a compelling parallel to the Contextual Maintenance and Retrieval (CMR) model that describes human episodic memory. This exploration adds a significant piece to the puzzle of understanding the intersection of artificial and biological intelligence systems.

Key Findings and Methodology

The paper's key goal is to demonstrate that induction heads in Transformer models exhibit behavioral and mechanistic similarities to the CMR model. The research unfolds through several methodical steps:

  1. Behavioral Parallels: The paper illustrates that induction heads in LLMs adopt behaviors similar to those seen in human episodic memory. Specifically, the attention mechanisms employed by induction heads to predict next tokens mirror episodic retrieval, marked by temporal contiguity and forward asymmetry. These phenomena are well-documented in human memory studies using the CMR framework.
  2. Mechanistic Similarities: By reinterpreting induction heads through the lens of the CMR model, the paper reveals that the computation performed by these attention heads can be likened to the associative retrieval processes in CMR. This includes mechanisms such as K-composition and Q-composition in induction heads, which align with the context and word retrieval operations in CMR.
  3. Empirical Validation: Through an analysis of pre-trained models like GPT-2 and Pythia, they demonstrate that induction heads with high induction-head matching scores exhibit attention biases consistent with CMR's model of human memory. The metrics used include CMR distance, allowing a quantitative assessment of the heads’ similarity to CMR behaviors.

Implications and Future Directions

This research offers meaningful insights for both AI and neuroscience fields. The characterized parallels allow us to rethink ICL mechanisms in LLMs, suggesting that these models may leverage processes analogous to human episodic memory to enhance next-token predictions. Such insights can drive the design of more sophisticated models with improved in-context learning capabilities and safer AI system behaviors.

For neuroscience, these findings contribute to our understanding of the hippocampal and cortico-hippocampal systems, potentially elucidating the function of these biological structures in similar computational tasks. The rediscovered normative principles underlying episodic memory biases offer valuable guidance for modeling human cognitive functions.

Practical Insight: Induction heads emerge predominantly in the intermediate layers of Transformer models, a discovery supported by examining both GPT-2 and Pythia models. This localized emergence can inform architecture designs optimized for better performance in language tasks and potentially other cognitive functions.

Theoretical Insight: The persistence of CMR-like behavior as model training progresses suggests that attention mechanisms in LLMs evolve in ways that reflect optimal memory recall strategies inherent in human cognition.

Speculative Future Developments: Future research may seek to confirm whether these findings generalize across other Transformer-based models and natural language settings. Additionally, exploring the "lost in the middle" phenomenon in deeper Transformers could reveal more about how these models manage long-range dependencies, echoing recency and primacy effects known in human memory.

Limitations

The current paper uses sequences of repeated random tokens to test induction behaviors, which might omit crucial aspects present in natural language processing tasks. Furthermore, whether CMR can serve as a fundamentally mechanistic model for the behavior of heads in extremely large Transformer models remains uncertain. Additional research should examine other variants of Transformer models to determine the robustness of these findings.

Conclusion

By establishing a bridge between the CMR model and induction heads in LLMs, the paper enriches our understanding of both artificial and biological systems. The alignment of Transformer attention mechanisms with human episodic memory models opens new pathways for cross-disciplinary research in AI and neuroscience, ultimately pushing towards more advanced and cognitively plausible models of intelligence.

Definition Search Book Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com
References (37)
  1. Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 111(23):8619–8624, 2014.
  2. Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex. Nature neuroscience, 19(3):356–365, 2016.
  3. Brain-score: Which artificial neural network for object recognition is most brain-like? BioRxiv, page 407007, 2018.
  4. Emergence of grid-like representations by training recurrent neural networks to perform spatial localization. arXiv preprint arXiv:1803.07770, 2018.
  5. Vector-based navigation using grid-like representations in artificial agents. Nature, 557(7705):429–433, 2018.
  6. Prefrontal cortex as a meta-reinforcement learning system. Nature neuroscience, 21(6):860–868, 2018.
  7. Attentional bias in human category learning: The case of deep learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2018. URL https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:4789239.
  8. Attention is all you need. In Isabelle Guyon, Ulrike von Luxburg, Samy Bengio, Hanna M. Wallach, Rob Fergus, S. V. N. Vishwanathan, and Roman Garnett, editors, Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 30: Annual Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems 2017, December 4-9, 2017, Long Beach, CA, USA, pages 5998–6008, 2017. URL https://proceedings.neurips.cc/paper/2017/hash/3f5ee243547dee91fbd053c1c4a845aa-Abstract.html.
  9. Alignment of brain embeddings and artificial contextual embeddings in natural language points to common geometric patterns. Nature communications, 15(1):2768, 2024.
  10. The neural architecture of language: Integrative reverse-engineering converges on a model for predictive processing. BioRxiv, 2020.
  11. Scaling laws for language encoding models in fmri. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 36, 2024.
  12. Relating transformers to models and neural representations of the hippocampal formation. arXiv preprint arXiv:2112.04035, 2021.
  13. In-context learning and induction heads. arXiv preprint arXiv:2209.11895, 2022.
  14. Language models are unsupervised multitask learners. OpenAI blog, 1(8):9, 2019.
  15. Language models are few-shot learners. Advances in neural information processing systems, 33:1877–1901, 2020.
  16. Murdock and B Bennet. The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64:482–488, 1962.
  17. A distributed representation of temporal context. Journal of mathematical psychology, 46(3):269–299, 2002.
  18. Does activation really spread? Psychological Review, 88(5):454–462, 1981. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.88.5.454.
  19. A mathematical framework for transformer circuits. Transformer Circuits Thread, 1, 2021.
  20. Gautam Reddy. The mechanistic basis of data dependence and abrupt learning in an in-context classification task. arXiv preprint arXiv:2312.03002, 2023.
  21. What needs to go right for an induction head? a mechanistic study of in-context learning circuits and their formation. arXiv preprint arXiv:2404.07129, 2024.
  22. Neel Nanda. Transformerlens, 2022. URL https://github.com/neelnanda-io/TransformerLens.
  23. A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. Psychological review, 116(1):129, 2009.
  24. Expanding the scope of memory search: Modeling intralist and interlist effects in free recall. Psychological review, 122:337–363, 04 2015a. doi: 10.1037/a0039036.
  25. Retrieved-context theory of memory in emotional disorders. bioRxiv, page 817486, 2019.
  26. The temporal context model in spatial navigation and relational learning: toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function across domains. Psychological review, 112 1:75–116, 2005. URL https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:16459919.
  27. The successor representation and temporal context. Neural Computation, 24(6):1553–1568, 2012.
  28. Episodic retrieval for model-based evaluation in sequential decision tasks, 2023. URL https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3sqjh.
  29. Rethinking the role of scale for in-context learning: An interpretability-based case study at 66 billion scale. In Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 2022.
  30. Optimal policies for free recall. Psychological Review, 130(4):1104–1124, 2023. doi: 10.1037/rev0000375.
  31. A context-based theory of recency and contiguity in free recall. Psychological review, 115 4:893–912, 2008.
  32. Expanding the scope of memory search: Modeling intralist and interlist effects in free recall. Psychological review, 122(2):337–63, 2015b.
  33. A predictive framework for evaluating models of semantic organization in free recall. Journal of memory and language, 86:119–140, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.10.002.
  34. Lost in the middle: How language models use long contexts. Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 12:157–173, 2024.
  35. The hippocampus as a predictive map. Nature neuroscience, 20(11):1643–1653, 2017.
  36. During running in place, grid cells integrate elapsed time and distance run. Neuron, 88(3):578–589, 2015.
  37. Building transformers from neurons and astrocytes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(34):e2219150120, 2023.
User Edit Pencil Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com
Authors (4)
  1. Li Ji-An (4 papers)
  2. Corey Y. Zhou (1 paper)
  3. Marcus K. Benna (6 papers)
  4. Marcelo G. Mattar (9 papers)
Citations (3)
X Twitter Logo Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com
Youtube Logo Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com